Motor learning paradoxes

– and implications for teaching.

When we begin to take a closer look at learning and teaching, we are quickly faced with some paradoxical phenomena. The definition of a paradox is, a seemingly absurd or contradictory statement or proposition which when investigated may prove to be well founded or true. We are conditioned to a linear way of thinking, when it comes to teaching sports, perfect practice makes perfect, and more is better seem to be widely accepted slogans. However an increasing amount of empirical evidence are stating that the opposite is actually true, order out of chaos and less is more. We are faced with paradoxical phenomenon that at first can seem challenging to grasp as a teacher, but when examined closer can inform our teaching. In this article I will try to clarify a few of the paradoxes we are often faced with while teaching, and how to turn this understanding into valuable tools to inform our choices of teaching methods and tools.

paradox.png

Order out of chaos

The first paradoxical phenomenon is “order out of chaos”. Based on motor-learning research, the first paradoxical principle states that interference and variability embedded in the practice environment may facilitate lasting learning even though they may create setbacks for initial learning. Our understanding of this phenomena originates from Schmidt’s schema theory, that postulates;

practicing variations of a movement (variable practice) will be of benefit when the movement must later be recalled (retention) or when producing a novel form of movement (transfer) [Schmidt 1975].

Two established research findings related to this type of paradox are the contextual interference effect [Shea et al 1979] and the variability effect [Schmidt 1975, Moxley et al 1979].

 

Contextual interference

The contextual interference effect in motor learning refers to the interference that results from practicing a task within the concept of other tasks in a practice session. Several studies have shown that practice under conditions of high contextual interference, with a random practice order, degrades performance during acquisition trials, compared to low contextual interference conditions, with a blocked order, where practice is completed on one task before practice on another task is undertaken. In contrast to acquisition performance, random practice usually leads to more effective learning than blocked practice, as measured by retention and transfer tests [Timothy et al 1992].

The variability effect

Research findings show that less variability produces better short-term benefits while more variability is often associated with better long-term retention and transfer effects [Edwards et al 1985, Proteau et al 1994]. Random practice increases the difficulty level of performance, thus producing a temporary setback in performance, but it may increase long-term retention rates [Shea et al 1979, Lin et al 2008, Lin et al 2009] .

blockedvsrandom.png

Opposing arguments?

There exist two opposing arguments in the above paradoxical phenomena, contextual interference and the variability effect.

  • One argument states that increased contextual interference or variability decreases initial performance.
  • The second, opposing argument states that increased contextual interference or variability increases lasting retention.

These counter intuitive findings are perplexing to most of us who are conditioned to linear thinking patterns, such as “what starts well ends well.” To apply this principle in teaching, it is important to remember that the level of interference and variability should be compatible with age, skill level, time allotted for practice, and instructional resources. Knowing the pros and cons of each argument in the paradox allows the student and teacher to move to the next stage of resolving the paradox. High contextual interference resulting from introducing multiple tasks and placing high variability demands on students for quick reactions tend to favor older, more skilled students who are provided with a lot of support [Chen 2011].

To increase the amount of contextual interference or chaos, the teacher may increase the degree of unpredictability in presenting multiple skills during practice (i.e., random practice). To lower contextual interference, the teacher can use a blocked practice schedule in which only one skill is drilled over and over until it is mastered before moving on to another skill [Chen 2011].

As an example, blocked practice may be instrumental for beginners to develop a stable pattern of movement, but it may also prevent students from moving to the next stage of expertise. Random practice is difficult for beginners who have not mastered the mechanics of basic skills, but it may promote specificity of training for highly skilled athletes.

Less is more..

The second paradoxical phenomena is “Less is more” as with “order out of chaos” it is a perplexing phenomena for most teachers. The main responsibility of teachers and coaches is to provide guidance for students and athletes in the form of verbal instructions, and feedback about the results of their performances. Guidance normally conjures up positive connotations, for it is associated with improvement. “More is better” is often the motto. We want our students to perform well, so the temptation is to provide them with as much guidance as possible. However, motor-learning research has proven that excessive guidance harms long-term learning, while reduced guidance can facilitate it [Salomi et al 1984]. In my experience with coaching teachers this is often where I see less experienced teachers struggle the most, they are so eager to help their students succeed that they forget this important paradox and it’s implications.

Understanding paradoxes such as “less is more” can improve teaching effectiveness.

– David Chen

The guidance hypothesis

In my previous article on motivation I touched upon how adopting a more question based approach to teaching can help our students develop problem solving skills, promote motivation and increase autonomy. The guidance hypothesis [Schmidt 1991], which has been empirically supported, states that providing external feedback too frequently may lead to a student’s dependence on the feedback, resulting in failure to process his or her own internal feedback. Therefore, it is recommended that instructors reduce the amount of guidance as learning progresses [Chen 2011]. Switching from a deductive or direct instruction based approach to a more Inductive method of teaching is an effective strategy to reduce our students reliance on feedback, improve problem solving skills, and increase autonomy. A similar strategy involving a question based approach would be to make use of the coaching method. In coaching, fundamentally, the coach is helping the individual to improve their own performance, and thus relies on a question based approach.

Coaching is unlocking a person’s potential to maximize their own performance. It is helping them to learn rather than teaching them.

– John Whitmore, in Coaching for Performance.

Good coaches believe that the individual always has the answer to their own problems but understands that they may need help to find the answer.

The benefits of reducing guidance for motor learning have been empirically verified and can take the following forms:

  • (1) decrease the frequency of feedback, and support the students in finding the answer to their own problems.
  • (2) delay the time interval between performance and feedback, or transition to posing questions that forces students to tune in to their internal feedback.
  • (3) provide more autonomy for students to solve problems.

Decreased guidance forces students to tune in to their internal feedback and develop more effective strategies for coping with emerging problems in the learning environment [Chen 2011].

Creative paradoxical thinking – summary

If we engage in a bit of creative paradoxical thinking, we can couple the above motor learning paradoxes with different methods of teaching, providing us with a clear guide of when and how to choose between our different teaching methods and tools. For example with beginners we can choose to use a direct instruction, frequent feedback, and blocked practice approach, to support our students initial acquisition. As the learning progresses, we can transition to a inductive approach, decrease the frequency of our feedback, and use random practice with a high contextual interference, to support retention and transfer for our more experienced students.

Understanding the implications of these paradoxes , order out of chaos and less is more can help inform our teaching, if we start paying close attention to our students progress we can choose the right approach at the right time.

  • Using blocked practice to help beginners develop stable movement patterns,
  • Random practice to promote specificity of training for more experienced students.
  • Decreasing our guidance and adopting a question based approach, to help more experienced students progress and develop their problem solving skills.

By doing so, we create an optimal learning environment for our students by carefully matching their experience, skill level and our instructional resources to their learning conditions.

References

  • Chen, 2011. Chen, David. 2011. Creative paradoxical thinking and its implications for teaching and learning
    motor skills. 82(11), 19.
  • Edwards & Lee, 1985. Edwards, R. V, & Lee, T.D. 1985. The relationship of cognitive style and instructional
    strategy to learning and transfer of motor skills. 56, 286–290.
  • Lee et al., 1992. Lee, Timothy D., Wulf, Gabriele, & Schmidt, Richard A. 1992. Contextual interference
    in motor learning: Dissociated effects due to the nature of task variations. The quarterly journal of experimental
    psychology section a, 44(4), 627–644.
  • Lin et al., 2008. Lin, Chien-Ho (Janice), Fisher, Beth E., Winstein, Carolee J., Wu, Allan D., &
    Gordon, James. 2008. Contextual interference effect: Elaborative processing or forgetting—reconstruction? a
    post hoc analysis of transcranial magnetic stimulation—induced effects on motor learning. Journal of motor
    behavior, 40(6), 578–586.
  • Lin et al., 2009. Lin, Chien-Ho (Janice), Fisher, Beth E., Wu, Allan D., Ko, Yi-An, Lee, Lung-Yee, &
    Winstein, Carolee J. 2009. Neural correlate of the contextual interference effect in motor learning: A
    kinematic analysis. Journal of motor behavior, 41(3), 232–242.
  • Moxley, 1979. Moxley, S.E. 1979. Schema. Journal of motor behavior, 11(1), 65–70. PMID: 15186973.
    Proteau et al., 1994. Proteau, L., Blandin, Y., Alain, C., & Dorion, A. 1994. The effects of the amount
    and variability of practice on the learning of a multi-segmented motor task. Acta psychol (amst), 85(1), 61–74.
    [PubMed:8165923].
  • Salmoni et al., 1984. Salmoni, A. W., Schmidt, R. A., & Walter, C. B. 1984. Knowledge of results and
    motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. Psychol bull, 95(3), 355–386. [PubMed:6399752].
    Schmidt, 1975. Schmidt, Richard A. 1975. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological
    review, 82(4), 225–260.
  • Schmidt, 1991. Schmidt, Richard A. 1991. Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence and
    interpretations. Pages 59–75 of: Tutorials in motor neuroscience. Springer Netherlands.
  • Shea & Morgan, 1979. Shea, John B., & Morgan, Robyn L. 1979. Contextual interference effects on the
    acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning &
    memory, 5(2), 179–187.

Acknowledgments

This article is inspired by David Chen’s 2011 article Creative paradoxical thinking and it’s implications for teaching and learning I highly recommend anyone interested in these paradoxes to check it out!

About the author

blank Master of Science in Sports, Martin Kvist.

Partner Acrobatics Teacher Trainer.

Highly educated in the field of sport science, neuroscience, and motor learning. Martin’s ability to provide injury prevention support, inspire healthy movement, and passion for acrobatics is essential for students who want to delve deep into their bodies for maximum strength and healing.

This entry was posted in Uncategorized. Bookmark the permalink.
BACK